Why Rational People Buy Into Conspiracy Theories ( New York Times )
In the days following the bombings at the Boston Marathon, speculation
online regarding the identity and motive of the unknown perpetrator or
perpetrators was rampant. And once the Tsarnaev brothers were identified
and the manhunt came to a close, the speculation didn’t cease. It took a
new form. A sampling: Maybe the brothers Tsarnaev were just patsies,
fall guys set up to take the heat for a mysterious Saudi with high-level
connections; or maybe they were innocent, but instead of the Saudis,
the actual bomber had acted on behalf of a rogue branch of our own
government; or what if the Tsarnaevs were behind the attacks, but were
secretly working for a larger organization?
Crazy as these theories are, those propagating them are not — they’re
quite normal, in fact. But recent scientific research tells us this
much: if you think one of the theories above is plausible, you probably
feel the same way about the others, even though they contradict one
another. And it’s very likely that this isn’t the only news story that
makes you feel as if shadowy forces are behind major world events.
“The best predictor of belief in a conspiracy theory is belief in other
conspiracy theories,” says Viren Swami, a psychology professor who
studies conspiracy belief at the University of Westminster in England.
Psychologists say that’s because a conspiracy theory isn’t so much a
response to a single event as it is an expression of an overarching
worldview.
As Richard Hofstadter wrote in his seminal 1965 book, “The Paranoid
Style in American Politics,” conspiracy theories, especially those
involving meddlesome foreigners, are a favorite pastime in this nation.
Americans have always had the sneaking suspicion that somebody was out
to get us — be it Freemasons, Catholics or communists. But in recent
years, it seems as if every tragedy comes with a round of yarn-spinning,
as the Web fills with stories about “false flag” attacks and “crisis
actors” — not mere theorizing but arguments for the existence of a
completely alternate version of reality.
Since Hofstadter’s book was published, our access to information has
vastly improved, which you would think would have helped minimize such
wild speculation. But according to recent scientific research on the
matter, it most likely only serves to make theories more convincing to
the public. What’s even more surprising is that this sort of theorizing
isn’t limited to those on the margins. Perfectly sane minds possess an
incredible capacity for developing narratives, and even some of the
wildest conspiracy theories can be grounded in rational thinking, which
makes them that much more pernicious. Consider this: 63 percent of
registered American voters believe in at least one political conspiracy
theory, according to a recent poll conducted by Fairleigh Dickinson
University.
While psychologists can’t know exactly what goes on
inside our heads, they have, through surveys and laboratory studies,
come up with a set of traits that correlate well with conspiracy belief.
In 2010, Swami and a co-author summarized this research in The
Psychologist, a scientific journal. They found, perhaps surprisingly,
that believers are more likely to be cynical about the world in general
and politics in particular. Conspiracy theories also seem to be more
compelling to those with low self-worth, especially with regard to their
sense of agency in the world at large. Conspiracy theories appear to be
a way of reacting to uncertainty and powerlessness.
Economic recessions, terrorist attacks and natural disasters are
massive, looming threats, but we have little power over when they occur
or how or what happens afterward. In these moments of powerlessness and
uncertainty, a part of the brain called the amygdala kicks into action.
Paul Whalen, a scientist at Dartmouth College who studies the amygdala,
says it doesn’t exactly do anything on its own. Instead, the amygdala
jump-starts the rest of the brain into analytical overdrive — prompting
repeated reassessments of information in an attempt to create a coherent
and understandable narrative, to understand what just happened, what
threats still exist and what should be done now. This may be a useful
way to understand how, writ large, the brain’s capacity for generating
new narratives after shocking events can contribute to so much paranoia
in this country.
0 comments:
Post a Comment
Welcome to www.soomronavi.blogspot.com